hosted by
publicationslist.org
    
Dilshat Abla

dabla@brain.riken.jp

Journal articles

2009
 
DOI   
PMID 
Dilshat Abla, Kazuo Okanoya (2009)  Visual statistical learning of shape sequences: an ERP study.   Neurosci Res 64: 2. 185-190 Jun  
Abstract: Behavioral experiments have found that infants and adults learn statistically defined patterns presented in auditory and visual input sequences in the same manner regardless of whether the input was linguistic (syllables) or nonlinguistic (tones and shapes). In order do determine the time course and neural processes involved in online word segmentation and statistical learning of visual sequence, we recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) while participants were exposed to continuous sequences with elements organized into shape-words randomly connected to each other. After viewing three 6.6min sessions of sequences, the participants performed a behavioral choice test. The participants were divided into two groups (high and low learners) based on their behavioral performance. The overall mean performance was 72.2%, indicating that the shape sequence was segmented and that the participants learned the shape-triplets statistically. Grand-averaged ERPs showed that triplet-onset (the initial shapes of shape-words) elicited larger N400 amplitudes than did middle and final shapes embedded in continuous streams during the early learning sessions of high learners, but no triplet-onset effect was found among low learners. The results suggested that the N400 effect indicated online segmentation of the visual sequence and the degree of statistical learning. Our results also imply that statistical learning represents a common learning device.
Notes:
2008
 
DOI   
PMID 
Kentaro Katahira, Dilshat Abla, Sayaka Masuda, Kazuo Okanoya (2008)  Feedback-based error monitoring processes during musical performance: an ERP study.   Neurosci Res 61: 1. 120-128 May  
Abstract: Auditory feedback is important in detecting and correcting errors during sound production when a current performance is compared to an intended performance. In the context of vocal production, a forward model, in which a prediction of action consequence (corollary discharge) is created, has been proposed to explain the dampened activity of the auditory cortex while producing self-generated vocal sounds. However, it is unclear how auditory feedback is processed and what neural mechanism underlies the process during other sound production behavior, such as musical performances. We investigated the neural correlates of human auditory feedback-based error detection using event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded during musical performances. Keyboard players of two different skill levels played simple melodies using a musical score. During the performance, the auditory feedback was occasionally altered. Subjects with early and extensive piano training produced a negative ERP component N210, which was absent in non-trained players. When subjects listened to music that deviated from a corresponding score without playing the piece, N210 did not emerge but the imaginary mismatch negativity (iMMN) did. Therefore, N210 may reflect a process of mismatch between the intended auditory image evoked by motor activity, and actual auditory feedback.
Notes:
 
DOI   
PMID 
Dilshat Abla, Kentaro Katahira, Kazuo Okanoya (2008)  On-line Assessment of Statistical Learning by Event-related Potentials.   J Cogn Neurosci 20: 6. 952-964 Jun  
Abstract: Abstract We investigated the neural processes involved in on-line statistical learning and word segmentation. Auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded while participants were exposed to continuous, nonlinguistic auditory sequences, the elements of which were organized into "tritone words" that were sequenced in random order, with no silent spaces between them. After listening to three 6.6-min sessions of sequences, the participants performed a behavioral choice test, in which they were instructed to indicate the most familiar tone sequence in each test trial by pressing buttons. The participants were divided into three groups (high, middle, and low learners) based on their behavioral performance. The overall mean performance was 74.4%, indicating that the tone sequence was segmented and that the participants learned the tone words statistically. Grand-averaged ERPs showed that word onset (initial tone) elicited the largest N100 and N400 in the early learning session of high learners, but in middle learners, the word-onset effect was elicited in a later session, and there was no effect in low learners. The N400 amplitudes significantly differed between the three learning sessions in the high- and middle-learner groups. The results suggest that the N400 effect indicates not only on-line word segmentation but also the degree of statistical learning. This study provides insight into the neural mechanisms underlying on-line statistical learning processes.
Notes:
 
DOI   
PMID 
Dilshat Abla, Kazuo Okanoya (2008)  Statistical segmentation of tone sequences activates the left inferior frontal cortex: a near-infrared spectroscopy study.   Neuropsychologia 46: 11. 2787-2795 Sep  
Abstract: Word segmentation, that is, discovering the boundaries between words that are embedded in a continuous speech stream, is an important faculty for language learners; humans solve this task partly by calculating transitional probabilities between sounds. Behavioral and ERP studies suggest that detection of sequential probabilities (statistical learning) plays an important role in the process of word segmentation. To identify the brain regions that are engaged during statistical segmentation of tone sequences, we measured changes in blood oxygenation using multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) while participants were exposed to continuous, nonlinguistic auditory sequences, the elements of which were organized into fixed "tone-words," but sequenced in random order. We habituated the participants to the tone-words by presenting the stimuli in a training phase prior to the presentation of the continuous tone sequences. After training, the statistical sequences, which included six tone-words, and the random sequences, which included 11 tones in random order, were presented alternately during NIRS recording. A large increase in the oxygenated hemoglobin concentration was observed in the left inferior frontal cortex (IFC) during the statistical sequence condition, but not during the random sequence condition. This suggests that the left IFC plays an important role in statistical segmentation of tone sequences, during which participants deploy the knowledge obtained in the training phase on the subsequent continuous sequence.
Notes:
2007
 
DOI   
PMID 
Naotaka Fujii, Dilshat Abla, Noriko Kudo, Sayaka Hihara, Kazuo Okanoya, Atsushi Iriki (2007)  Prefrontal activity during koh-do incense discrimination.   Neurosci Res 59: 3. 257-264 Nov  
Abstract: Whenever we make reasoned decisions we must refer to relevant knowledge obtained through past experience. Our brains test multiple premises and select whichever conclusion serves as the best explanation of the current conditions. In the present study we examined the prefrontal activity of koh-do experts with near infrared spectroscopy while they reasoned about odours during an incense discrimination task. These practitioners of the Japanese incense ceremony have been trained to form and manipulate abstract images of complex olfactory stimuli represented in a multidimensional symbolic space in the mind. In koh-do experts, the right PFC showed a consistent stimulus-non-selective response during discrimination and the left PFC showed phasic stimulus-selective responses modulated by the internal subjective state of the reasoning process. These two dissociated functions appeared to cooperate with each other during reasoning. In contrast, koh-do beginners did not show the organized response pattern found in experts. The results suggest that both PFCs contribute to abductive reasoning, but do so differently through different stages of the process.
Notes:
1999
 
PMID 
T Shibata, I Shimoyama, T Ito, D Abla, H Iwasa, K Koseki, N Yamanouchi, T Sato, Y Nakajima (1999)  Attention changes the peak latency of the visual gamma-band oscillation of the EEG.   Neuroreport 10: 6. 1167-1170 Apr  
Abstract: To investigate the physiological role of visual gamma-band oscillation (GBO), we calculated the event-related dynamics of the EEG power-spectrum for paired visual stimuli (S1 and S2) with or without attention in 12 subjects. The visual stimuli elicited transient increases in the GBO power (around 40 Hz), which were maximal over the parietal area. The peak GBO increase appeared around 300 ms after stimulus onset, but its latency was shorter after S1 and longer after S2 under the 'with attention' than under the 'without attention' condition. This transient increase in the visual GBO is thought to reflect attention and to reset the activity of the visual system in preparation for a new stimulus.
Notes:
 
PMID 
T Shibata, I Shimoyama, T Ito, D Abla, H Iwasa, K Koseki, N Yamanouchi, T Sato, Y Nakajima (1999)  Event-related dynamics of the gamma-band oscillation in the human brain: information processing during a GO/NOGO hand movement task.   Neurosci Res 33: 3. 215-222 Mar  
Abstract: To investigate the gamma band activity relating to the discrimination process and motor behavior in the human brain, the event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum was calculated during the visual GO/NOGO hand movement task and a control task (the visual element of the GO/NOGO task only) in eight subjects. The subjects were instructed to push (GO) or not to push (NOGO) a microswitch according to different visual stimuli and 21-channel scalp EEGs were recorded. The time courses of the power spectra after the stimuli were calculated using the fast Fourier transform for each condition (GO, NOGO and the control task), and were compared statistically between the conditions. The results suggested that a high gamma band oscillation, occurring at the frontal and left parieto-occipital areas at around 90 ms after the stimuli, relates to the discrimination process. Under the GO condition, this oscillation continued until 140 ms, and a subsequent oscillation occurred over the motor areas at around 200 ms, which seemed to be related to the motor action. On the other hand, under the NOGO condition, a low gamma band oscillation occurred in the central area at around 230 ms, which seemed to be related to the inhibition process.
Notes:
1998
 
PMID 
T Shibata, I Shimoyama, T Ito, D Abla, H Iwasa, K Koseki, N Yamanouchi, T Sato, Y Nakajima (1998)  The synchronization between brain areas under motor inhibition process in humans estimated by event-related EEG coherence.   Neurosci Res 31: 4. 265-271 Aug  
Abstract: To investigate the functional connection of brain areas under motor inhibition, the event-related coherence (ERCoh) of the electroencephalogram (EEG) was calculated for 10 subjects who were asked to perform a visual discrimination (GO/NO-GO) task. The subjects were instructed to push (GO) or not to push (NO-GO) a micro-switch according to different visual stimuli. Twenty-one-channel scalp EEGs were recorded and the surface Laplacians were calculated at F3, F4, C3, C4, P3 and P4 using the source derivation method. The time-courses of the inter- and intra-hemispheric coherence were calculated using the fast Fourier transform for each condition (GO or NO-GO), and were compared statistically between the two conditions. The results suggest that the ERCoh under the NO-GO condition consisted of two components; alpha band synchronization between bilateral frontal areas and theta band synchronization among bilateral frontal, central and parietal areas. It is likely that the former is related directly to the decision not to move, and the latter is related to the motor inhibition process.
Notes:
 
PMID 
I Shimoyama, Y Nakajima, T Shibata, T Ito, D Abla, K Kansaku, J Mizota (1998)  Binocular interactions in visual evoked cortical potentials with two light-emitting-diodes.   Doc Ophthalmol 97: 1. 1-7 1999  
Abstract: Binocular interaction for a central field was studied with transient scalp visual evoked cortical potentials (VECPs) using two light-emitting-diodes. VECPs were obtained for binocular and monocular visions with dominant and non-dominant eyes, and arithmetical sums of monocular VECPs with dominant and non-dominant eyes were calculated. Amplitude and latency of remarkable initial three peaks were tested with the multivariate analysis of variance. Significant differences were noted among the four VECPs. Pairwise comparisons showed that (1) the amplitude of the first peak for the binocular VECPs was larger than that for the monocular VECPs but smaller than that for the sum-VECPs; the latency of the first peaks for the binocular VECPs were earlier than that for the monocular VECPs with the non-dominant eye; (2) the amplitude of the first negative peak for the sum-VECPs was larger than that for the binocular VECPs, and the peak latency for the sum-VECPs showed later than that for the binocular VECPs; (3) the amplitude of the second positive peak for the binocular VECPs and monocular VECPs with the dominant eye was larger than that with the non-dominant eye, but smaller for the binocular VECPS than that for the sum-VECPs; the latency for the binocular VECPs showed earlier than that for the monocular VECPs with the dominant eye and for the sum-VECPs. Binocular suppression was noted in amplitude for the three peaks and binocular facilitation was noted in latency for the latter two peaks.
Notes:
 
PMID 
I Shimoyama, T Ito, T Shibata, D Abla, Y Nakajima (1998)  Hemispheric laterality in contextual recognition.   Jpn J Physiol 48: 6. 493-497 Dec  
Abstract: Hemispheric dominance in contextual recognition is now under discussion. Auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) with a contextual decision task were recorded to study hemispheric dominance by using the wave energy and amplitudes of N400 for eight native volunteers. Two types of 80 sentences with four clauses were used as the structure: [subject] + [object or complement] + [object] + [two-syllable verb]. One type was congruent and the other deviated at the terminal clause as an incongruent sentence. Twenty-one channel ERPs were triggered at the verbs and recorded during -200 and 1,000 ms, with a noncephalic reference from 0.05 to 30 Hz. The wave energy was calculated as mean square potentials and was normalized with the background activity (pretrigger 200 ms). The amplitudes of N400 showed significant differences between the incongruent and congruent sentences, but the hemispheric dominance for the amplitudes of N400 was not significant between the incongruent and congruent sentences. Reaction times with the index finger to respond to the deviated verbs were 470.0 (mean) +/- 42.5 (SD) ms. The maximum normalized wave energy within the mean reaction time showed significant differences between the incongruent and congruent sentences, and the left hemispheric energy was shown to be significantly higher than the right one.
Notes:
1997
 
PMID 
T Shibata, I Shimoyama, T Ito, D Abla, H Iwasa, K Koseki, N Yamanouchi, T Sato, Y Nakajima (1997)  The time course of interhemispheric EEG coherence during a GO/NO-GO task in humans.   Neurosci Lett 233: 2-3. 117-120 Sep  
Abstract: Event-related coherence of the EEG was calculated for 10 subjects performing a visual discrimination GO/NO-GO task. The subjects were instructed to push (GO) or not to push (NO-GO) a button according to visual stimuli. Twenty-one-channel scalp EEGs were recorded and the surface Laplacian was calculated using the source derivation method. The time courses of the coherence between F3 and F4, C3 and C4, and P3 and P4 were calculated using the fast Fourier transform for each task and were compared between conditions. Statistical analysis showed that coherence in the NO-GO condition became significantly higher than that in the GO condition between F3 and F4. The synchronization between bilateral dorsolateral frontal areas might therefore play an important role in the motor inhibition process.
Notes:
1990
 
PMID 
Y Tanaka, D Abla, N Hirai (1990)  Activity of crossed spinocerebellar tract neurones in the thoracic spinal cord in relation to the central respiratory rhythm.   Brain Res 532: 1-2. 339-341 Nov  
Abstract: Correlation between respiratory movement and neural discharges of antidromically identified 20 crossed spinocerebellar tract (SCT) neurones in the T8-11 segments was examined in the anaesthetized, paralysed cat. Activity of 11 neurones of them was related to phrenic nerve activity; 7 neurones increased and 4 decreased their activity during the active phase of the phrenic nerve. Their rhythmic activity remained even after artificial ventilation was turned off, indicating the central genesis of their rhythmic activity. By comparing discharge patterns with the ventilator on and off, it is suggested that half of these crossed SCT neurones appear to receive additional inputs from the periphery. Neurones which increased their firing rate during phrenic nerve activity tend to respond with decrease discharge to passive chest inflation, and vise versa.
Notes:
Powered by publicationslist.org.