hosted by
publicationslist.org
    

Driss Boussaoud


driss.boussaoud@univ-amu.fr

Journal articles

2012
Fadila Hadj-Bouziane, Isabelle Benatru, Andrea Brovelli, HĂ©lène Klinger, StĂ©phane Thobois, Emmanuel Broussolle, Driss Boussaoud, Martine Meunier (2012)  Advanced Parkinson's disease effect on goal-directed and habitual processes involved in visuomotor associative learning.   Front Hum Neurosci 6: 01  
Abstract: The present behavioral study re-addresses the question of habit learning in Parkinson's disease (PD). Patients were early onset, non-demented, dopa-responsive, candidates for surgical treatment, similar to those we found earlier as suffering greater dopamine depletion in the putamen than in the caudate nucleus. The task was the same conditional associative learning task as that used previously in monkeys and healthy humans to unveil the striatum involvement in habit learning. Sixteen patients and 20 age- and education-matched healthy control subjects learned sets of 3 visuo-motor associations between complex patterns and joystick displacements during two testing sessions separated by a few hours. We distinguished errors preceding vs. following the first correct response to compare patients' performance during the earliest phase of learning dominated by goal-directed actions with that observed later on, when responses start to become habitual. The disease significantly retarded both learning phases, especially in patients under 60 years of age. However, only the late phase deficit was disease severity-dependent and persisted on the second testing session. These findings provide the first corroboration in Parkinson patients of two ideas well-established in the animal literature. The first is the idea that associating visual stimuli to motor acts is a form of habit learning that engages the striatum. It is confirmed here by the global impairment in visuo-motor learning induced by PD. The second idea is that goal-directed behaviors are predominantly caudate-dependent whereas habitual responses are primarily putamen-dependent. At the advanced PD stages tested here, dopamine depletion is greater in the putamen than in the caudate nucleus. Accordingly, the late phase of learning corresponding to the emergence of habitual responses was more vulnerable to the disease than the early phase dominated by goal-directed actions.
Notes:
Elisabetta Monfardini, ValĂ©rie Gaveau, Driss Boussaoud, Fadila Hadj-Bouziane, Martine Meunier (2012)  Social learning as a way to overcome choice-induced preferences? Insights from humans and rhesus macaques.   Front Neurosci 6: 09  
Abstract: Much theoretical attention is currently devoted to social learning. Yet, empirical studies formally comparing its effectiveness relative to individual learning are rare. Here, we focus on free choice, which is at the heart of individual reward-based learning, but absent in social learning. Choosing among two equally valued options is known to create a preference for the selected option in both humans and monkeys. We thus surmised that social learning should be more helpful when choice-induced preferences retard individual learning than when they optimize it. To test this prediction, the same task requiring to find which among two items concealed a reward was applied to rhesus macaques and humans. The initial trial was individual or social, rewarded or unrewarded. Learning was assessed on the second trial. Choice-induced preference strongly affected individual learning. Monkeys and humans performed much more poorly after an initial negative choice than after an initial positive choice. Comparison with social learning verified our prediction. For negative outcome, social learning surpassed or at least equaled individual learning in all subjects. For positive outcome, the predicted superiority of individual learning did occur in a majority of subjects (5/6 monkeys and 6/12 humans). A minority kept learning better socially though, perhaps due to a more dominant/aggressive attitude toward peers. Poor learning from errors due to over-valuation of personal choices is among the decision-making biases shared by humans and animals. The present study suggests that choice-immune social learning may help curbing this potentially harmful tendency. Learning from successes is an easier path. The present data suggest that whether one tends to walk it alone or with a peer's help might depend on the social dynamics within the actor/observer dyad.
Notes:
Abdelhak Mahmoudi, Sylvain Takerkart, Fakhita Regragui, Driss Boussaoud, Andrea Brovelli (2012)  Multivoxel pattern analysis for FMRI data: a review.   Comput Math Methods Med 2012: 12  
Abstract: Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) exploits blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) contrasts to map neural activity associated with a variety of brain functions including sensory processing, motor control, and cognitive and emotional functions. The general linear model (GLM) approach is used to reveal task-related brain areas by searching for linear correlations between the fMRI time course and a reference model. One of the limitations of the GLM approach is the assumption that the covariance across neighbouring voxels is not informative about the cognitive function under examination. Multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA) represents a promising technique that is currently exploited to investigate the information contained in distributed patterns of neural activity to infer the functional role of brain areas and networks. MVPA is considered as a supervised classification problem where a classifier attempts to capture the relationships between spatial pattern of fMRI activity and experimental conditions. In this paper , we review MVPA and describe the mathematical basis of the classification algorithms used for decoding fMRI signals, such as support vector machines (SVMs). In addition, we describe the workflow of processing steps required for MVPA such as feature selection, dimensionality reduction, cross-validation, and classifier performance estimation based on receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves.
Notes:
Mounir Ouzir, Jean Michel Azorin, Marc Adida, Driss Boussaoud, Omar Battas (2012)  Insight in schizophrenia: from conceptualization to neuroscience.   Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 66: 3. 167-179 Apr  
Abstract: Lack of insight into illness is a prevalent and distinguishing feature of schizophrenia, which has a complex history and has been given a variety of definitions. Currently, insight is measured and treated as a multidimensional phenomenon, because it is believed to result from psychological, neuropsychological and organic factors. Thus, schizophrenia patients may display dramatic disorders including demoralization, depression and a higher risk of suicide, all of which are directly or indirectly related to a lack of insight into their illness, and make the treatment difficult. To improve the treatment of people with schizophrenia, it is thus crucial to advance research on insight into their illness. Insight is studied in a variety of ways. Studies may focus on the relationship between insight and psychopathology, may view behavioral outcomes or look discretely at the cognitive dysfunction versus anatomy level of insight. All have merit but they are dispersed across a wide body of literature and rarely are the findings integrated and synthesized in a meaningful way. The aim of this study was to synthesize findings across the large body of literature dealing with insight, to highlight its multidimensional nature, measurement, neuropsychology and social impact in schizophrenia. The extensive literature on the cognitive consequences of lack of insight and the contribution of neuroimaging techniques to elucidating neurological etiology of insight deficits, is also reviewed.
Notes:
2011
David Thura, Fadila Hadj-Bouziane, Martine Meunier, Driss Boussaoud (2011)  Hand modulation of visual, preparatory, and saccadic activity in the monkey frontal eye field.   Cereb Cortex 21: 4. 853-864 Apr  
Abstract: Behavioral studies have shown that hand position influences saccade characteristics. This study examined the neuronal changes that could underlie this behavioral observation. Single neurons were recorded in the frontal eye field (FEF) of 2 monkeys as they executed a visually guided saccade task, while holding their hand at given locations on a touch screen. The task was performed with the hand either visible or invisible, in order to assess the relative contribution of visual and proprioceptive information on hand position. Among the 224 neurons tested, the visual, saccadic and/or preparatory activity of more than half of them was modulated by hand position, whether the hand was visible or invisible. Comparison of lower (hand's workspace) and upper (out of reach) visual targets showed that hand modulation was predominant in the hand's workspace. Finally, some cells preferred congruency of hand and target in space, others preferred incongruency. Interestingly, hand modulation of saccadic activity correlated with hand position effects on saccade reaction times. We conclude that visual and proprioceptive signals derived from the hand are integrated by FEF neurons. These signals can modulate target selection through attention and allow the oculomotor system to use hand-related somatosensory signals for the initiation of visually guided saccades.
Notes:
Andrea Brovelli, Bruno Nazarian, Martine Meunier, Driss Boussaoud (2011)  Differential roles of caudate nucleus and putamen during instrumental learning.   Neuroimage 57: 4. 1580-1590 Aug  
Abstract: The dorsal striatum is crucial for the acquisition and consolidation of instrumental behaviour, but the underlying computations and internal dynamics remain elusive. To address this issue, we combined a model of key computations supporting decision-making during instrumental learning with human behavioural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data. The results showed that the associative and sensorimotor dorsal striatum host complementary computations that, we suggest, may differentially support goal-directed and habitual processes. The anterior caudate nucleus integrates information about performance and cognitive control demands, whereas the putamen tracks how likely the conditioning stimuli lead to correct response. Contrary to current models, the putamen is recruited during initial acquisition. As the exploratory phase proceeds, the relative contribution of the caudate nucleus becomes dominant over the putamen. During early consolidation, caudate nucleus and putamen settle to asymptotic values and share control. We then investigated how dorsal striatal computations may affect decision-making. We found that portion of reaction times' variance parallels the combined cost associated with the dorsal striatal computations. Overall, our findings provide a deeper insight into the functional heterogeneity within the dorsal striatum and suggest that the dynamic interplay between caudate nucleus and putamen, rather than their serial recruitment, underlies the acquisition and early consolidation of instrumental behaviours.
Notes:
2008
Elisabetta Monfardini, Andrea Brovelli, Driss Boussaoud, Sylvain Takerkart, Bruno Wicker (2008)  I learned from what you did: Retrieving visuomotor associations learned by observation.   Neuroimage 42: 3. 1207-1213 Sep  
Abstract: Observational learning allows individuals to acquire knowledge without incurring in the costs and risks of discovering and testing. The neural mechanisms mediating the retrieval of rules learned by observation are currently unknown. To explore this fundamental cognitive ability, we compared the brain responses when retrieving visuomotor associations learned either by observation or by individual learning. To do so, we asked eleven adults to learn two sets of arbitrary visuomotor associations: one set was learned through the observation of an expert actor while the other was learned by trial and error. During fMRI scanning, subjects were requested to retrieve the visuomotor associations previously learned under the two modalities. The conjunction analysis between the two learning conditions revealed a common brain network that included the ventral and dorsal lateral prefrontal cortices, the superior parietal lobe and the pre-SMA. This suggests the existence of a mirror-like system responsible for the storage of rules learned either by trial and error or by observation of others' actions. In addition, the pars triangularis in the right prefrontal cortex (BA45) was found to be selective for rules learned by observation. This suggests a preferential role of this area in the storage of rules learned in a social context.
Notes:
Nadia Allami, Yves Paulignan, Andrea Brovelli, Driss Boussaoud (2008)  Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice.   Exp Brain Res 184: 1. 105-113 Jan  
Abstract: Sports psychology suggests that mental rehearsal facilitates physical practice in athletes and clinical rehabilitation attempts to use mental rehearsal to restore motor function in hemiplegic patients. Our aim was to examine whether mental rehearsal is equivalent to physical learning, and to determine the optimal proportions of real execution and rehearsal. Subjects were asked to grasp an object and insert it into an adapted slot. One group (G0) practiced the task only by physical execution (240 trials); three groups imagined performing the task in different rates of trials (25%, G25; 50%, G50; 75%, G75), and physically executed movements for the remaining trials; a fourth, control group imagined a visual rotation task in 75% of the trials and then performed the same motor task as the others groups. Movement time (MT) was compared for the first and last physical trials, together with other key trials, across groups. All groups learned, suggesting that mental rehearsal is equivalent to physical motor learning. More importantly, when subjects rehearsed the task for large numbers of trials (G50 and G75), the MT of the first executed trial was significantly shorter than the first executed trial in the physical group (G0), indicating that mental practice is better than no practice at all. Comparison of the first executed trial in G25, G50 and G75 with the corresponding trials in G0 (61, 121 and 181 trials), showed equivalence between mental and physical practice. At the end of training, the performance was much better with high rates of mental practice (G50/G75) compared to physical practice alone (G0), especially when the task was difficult. These findings confirm that mental rehearsal can be beneficial for motor learning and suggest that imagery might be used to supplement or partly replace physical practice in clinical rehabilitation.
Notes:
David Thura, Driss Boussaoud, Martine Meunier (2008)  Hand position affects saccadic reaction times in monkeys and humans.   J Neurophysiol 99: 5. 2194-2202 May  
Abstract: In daily life, activities requiring the hand and eye to work separately are as frequent as activities requiring tight eye-hand coordination, and we effortlessly switch from one type of activity to the other. Such flexibility is unlikely to be achieved without each effector "knowing" where the other one is at all times, even when it is static. Here, we provide behavioral evidence that the mere position of the static hand affects one eye movement parameter: saccadic reaction time. Two monkeys were trained and 11 humans instructed to perform nondelayed or delayed visually guided saccades to either a right or a left target while holding their hand at a location either near or far from the eye target. From trial to trial, target locations and hand positions varied pseudorandomly. Subjects were tested both when they could and when they could not see their hand. The main findings are 1) the presence of the static hand in the workspace did affect saccade initiation; 2) this interaction persisted when the hand was invisible; 3) it was strongly influenced by the delay duration: hand-target proximity retarded immediate saccades, whereas it could hasten delayed saccades; and 4) this held true both for humans and for each of the two monkeys. We propose that both visual and nonvisual hand position signals are used by the primates' oculomotor system for the planning and execution of saccades, and that this may result in a hand-eye competition for spatial attentional resources that explains the delay-dependent reversal observed.
Notes:
Andrea Brovelli, Nadia Laksiri, Bruno Nazarian, Martine Meunier, Driss Boussaoud (2008)  Understanding the neural computations of arbitrary visuomotor learning through fMRI and associative learning theory.   Cereb Cortex 18: 7. 1485-1495 Jul  
Abstract: Associative theory postulates that learning the consequences of our actions in a given context is represented in the brain as stimulus-response-outcome associations that evolve according to prediction-error signals (the discrepancy between the observed and predicted outcome). We tested the theory on brain functional magnetic resonance imaging data acquired from human participants learning arbitrary visuomotor associations. We developed a novel task that systematically manipulated learning and induced highly reproducible performances. This granted the validation of the model-based results and an in-depth analysis of the brain signals in representative single trials. Consistent with the Rescorla-Wagner model, prediction-error signals are computed in the human brain and selectively engage the ventral striatum. In addition, we found evidence of computations not formally predicted by the Rescorla-Wagner model. The dorsal fronto-parietal network, the dorsal striatum, and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex are activated both on the incorrect and first correct trials and may reflect the processing of relevant visuomotor mappings during the early phases of learning. The left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is selectively activated on the first correct outcome. The results provide quantitative evidence of the neural computations mediating arbitrary visuomotor learning and suggest new directions for future computational models.
Notes:
David Thura, Fadila Hadj-Bouziane, Martine Meunier, Driss Boussaoud (2008)  Hand position modulates saccadic activity in the frontal eye field.   Behav Brain Res 186: 1. 148-153 Jan  
Abstract: Recent neurophysiological studies have begun to uncover the neuronal correlates of eye hand coordination. This study was designed to test whether the frontal eye field (FEF) saccadic activity is modulated by hand position. Single neurons were recorded in two macaque monkeys performing visually guided saccades while holding their hand at given locations on a touchscreen. To determine the relative contributions of hand vision and its proprioception, monkeys executed the task with or without vision of the hand. We found that saccadic activity of more than half of the neuronal sample (54%; n=130) was dependent on hand position relative to the saccade end point. Both visual and proprioceptive signals contributed to this modulation. These data demonstrate that the oculomotor function of the FEF takes into account hand position in space.
Notes:
2007
Martine Meunier, Elisabetta Monfardini, Driss Boussaoud (2007)  Learning by observation in rhesus monkeys.   Neurobiol Learn Mem 88: 2. 243-248 Sep  
Abstract: Habit memory provides us with a vast repertoire of learned rules, including stimulus-reward associations, that ensures fast and adapted decision making in daily life. Because we share this ability with monkeys, lesion and recording studies in rhesus macaques have played a key role in understanding the neural bases of individual trial-and-error habit learning. Humans, however, can learn new rules at a lower cost via observation of conspecifics. The neural properties underlying this more ecological form of habit learning remain unexplored, and it is unclear whether the rhesus macaque can be a useful model in this endeavor. We addressed this issue by testing four monkeys from the same social group in their usual semi-natural habitat using a well-established marker of habit memory, concurrent discrimination learning. Each monkey learned 24 lists of 10 object-reward associations each. For one list out of two, monkeys could observe the testing session of another member of the group prior to being tested with the same list themselves. Learning was faster for these lists than for those learned solely by trial-and-error. Errors to criterion (9/10 correct responses) were reduced by 39%, and faultless performance could be achieved for up to 5 of the 10 pairs. These data demonstrate that rhesus macaques spontaneously observe a conspecific learning new stimulus-reward associations, and substantially benefit from this observation. They ascertain that the neural underpinnings of socially-mediated forms of habit learning can be explored using the powerful tools of monkey research, including neurophysiological recordings.
Notes:
Andrea Brovelli, Pierre-Arnaud Coquelin, Driss Boussaoud (2007)  Estimating the hidden learning representations.   J Physiol Paris 101: 1-3. 110-117 Jan/May  
Abstract: Successful adaptation relies on the ability to learn the consequence of our actions in different environments. However, understanding the neural bases of this ability still represents one of the great challenges of system neuroscience. In fact, the neuronal plasticity changes occurring during learning cannot be fully controlled experimentally and their evolution is hidden. Our approach is to provide hypotheses about the structure and dynamics of the hidden plasticity changes using behavioral learning theory. In fact, behavioral models of animal learning provide testable predictions about the hidden learning representations by formalizing their relation with the observables of the experiment (stimuli, actions and outcomes). Thus, we can understand whether and how the predicted learning processes are represented at the neural level by estimating their evolution and correlating them with neural data. Here, we present a bayesian model approach to estimate the evolution of the internal learning representations from the observations of the experiment (state estimation), and to identify the set of models' parameters (parameter estimation) and the class of behavioral model (model selection) that are most likely to have generated a given sequence of actions and outcomes. More precisely, we use Sequential Monte Carlo methods for state estimation and the maximum likelihood principle (MLP) for model selection and parameter estimation. We show that the method recovers simulated trajectories of learning sessions on a single-trial basis and provides predictions about the activity of different categories of neurons that should participate in the learning process. By correlating the estimated evolutions of the learning variables, we will be able to test the validity of different models of instrumental learning and possibly identify the neural bases of learning.
Notes:
2006
Fadila Hadj-Bouziane, HĂ©lène Frankowska, Martine Meunier, Pierre-Arnaud Coquelin, Driss Boussaoud (2006)  Conditional visuo-motor learning and dimension reduction.   Cogn Process 7: 2. 95-104 Jun  
Abstract: Conditional visuo-motor learning consists in learning by trial and error to associate visual cues with correct motor responses, that have no direct link. Converging evidence supports the role of a large brain network in this type of learning, including the prefrontal and the premotor cortex, the basal ganglia BG and the hippocampus. In this paper we focus on the role of a major structure of the BG, the striatum. We first present behavioral results and electrophysiological data recorded from this structure in monkeys engaged in learning new visuo-motor associations. Visual stimuli were presented on a video screen and the animals had to learn, by trial and error, to select the correct movement of a joystick, in order to receive a liquid reward. Behavioral results revealed that the monkeys used a sequential strategy, whereby they learned the associations one by one although they were presented randomly. Human subjects, tested on the same task, also used a sequential strategy. Neuronal recordings in monkeys revealed learning-related modulations of neural activity in the striatum. We then present a mathematical model inspired by viability theory developed to implement the use of strategies during learning. This model complements existing models of the BG based on reinforcement learning RL, which do not take into account the use of strategies to reduce the dimension of the learning space.
Notes:
Alice C Roy, Yves Paulignan, Martine Meunier, Driss Boussaoud (2006)  Prehension movements in the macaque monkey: effects of perturbation of object size and location.   Exp Brain Res 169: 2. 182-193 Feb  
Abstract: While the neural bases of prehension have been extensively studied in monkeys, a few kinematic studies have examined their prehension behavior. Recently (Roy et al. 2000, 2002), we have described the kinematics of reaching and grasping in freely behaving monkeys under normal conditions by applying the high-resolution recording techniques (Optotrak system) and behavioral paradigms used in humans. Here we determined whether online movement reorganization observed in monkeys following sudden changes of either object size or location at movement onset is similar to that observed in humans. We found that changing object size led to rapid on-flight re-calibration of the different movement parameters, eventually preserving the unitary aspect of the movement with a minor time cost. By contrast, a shift in object location triggered a massive time-consuming reorganization. Re-directed movements appeared as a concatenation of two sub-movements: a first one directed to the initial object and a second one directed to the new object location. These findings first complement our earlier studies in providing further evidence of the similarities between monkey and human prehension. Second, they suggest that the two components of prehension, reaching and grasping, interact through coordination mechanisms that are more efficient to correct for size than for location perturbation. This difference may reflect a hierarchical organization in which reaching would be the subordinate of grasping in both primate species.
Notes:
2005
Andrea Brovelli, Jean-Philippe Lachaux, Philippe Kahane, Driss Boussaoud (2005)  High gamma frequency oscillatory activity dissociates attention from intention in the human premotor cortex.   Neuroimage 28: 1. 154-164 Oct  
Abstract: The premotor cortex is well known for its role in motor planning. In addition, recent studies have shown that it is also involved in nonmotor functions such as attention and memory, a notion derived from both animal neurophysiology and human functional imaging. The present study is an attempt to bridge the gap between these experimental techniques in the human brain, using a task initially designed to dissociate attention from intention in the monkey, and recently adapted for a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study [Simon, S.R., Meunier, M., Piettre, L., Berardi, A.M., Segebarth, C.M., Boussaoud, D. (2002). Spatial attention and memory versus motor preparation: premotor cortex involvement as revealed by fMRI. J. Neurophysiol., 88, 2047-57]. Intracranial EEG was recorded from the cortical regions preferentially active in the spatial attention and/or working memory task and those involved in motor intention. The results show that, among the different intracranial EEG responses, only the high gamma frequency (60-200 Hz) oscillatory activity both dissociates attention/memory from motor intention and spatially colocalizes with the fMRI-identified premotor substrates of these two functions. This finding provides electrophysiological confirmation that the human premotor cortex is involved in spatial attention and/or working memory. Additionally, it provides timely support to the idea that high gamma frequency oscillations are involved in the cascade of neural processes underlying the hemodynamic responses measured with fMRI [Logothetis, N.K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T. and Oeltermann, A. (2001). Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature, 412, 150-7], and suggests a functional selectivity of the gamma oscillations that could be critical for future EEG investigations, whether experimental or clinical.
Notes:
Driss Boussaoud, Judith TannĂ©-GariĂ©py, Thierry Wannier, Eric M Rouiller (2005)  Callosal connections of dorsal versus ventral premotor areas in the macaque monkey: a multiple retrograde tracing study.   BMC Neurosci 6: 11  
Abstract: BACKGROUND: The lateral premotor cortex plays a crucial role in visually guided limb movements. It is divided into two main regions, the dorsal (PMd) and ventral (PMv) areas, which are in turn subdivided into functionally and anatomically distinct rostral (PMd-r and PMv-r) and caudal (PMd-c and PMv-c) sub-regions. We analyzed the callosal inputs to these premotor subdivisions following 23 injections of retrograde tracers in eight macaque monkeys. In each monkey, 2-4 distinct tracers were injected in different areas allowing direct comparisons of callosal connectivity in the same brain. RESULTS: Based on large injections covering the entire extent of the corresponding PM area, we found that each area is strongly connected with its counterpart in the opposite hemisphere. Callosal connectivity with the other premotor areas, the primary motor cortex, prefrontal cortex and somatosensory cortex varied from one area to another. The most extensive callosal inputs terminate in PMd-r and PMd-c, with PMd-r strongly connected with prefrontal cortex. Callosal inputs to PMv-c are more extensive than those to PMv-r, whose connections are restricted to its counterpart area. Quantitative analysis of labelled cells confirms these general findings, and allows an assessment of the relative strength of callosal inputs. CONCLUSION: PMd-r and PMv-r receive their strongest callosal inputs from their respective counterpart areas, whereas PMd-c and PMv-c receive strong inputs from heterotopic areas as well (namely from PMd-r and PMv-r, respectively). Finally, PMd-r stands out as the lateral premotor area with the strongest inputs from the prefrontal cortex, and only the PMd-c and PMv-c receive weak callosal inputs from M1.
Notes:
2004
James M Kilner, Yves Paulignan, Driss Boussaoud (2004)  Functional connectivity during real vs imagined visuomotor tasks: an EEG study.   Neuroreport 15: 4. 637-642 Mar  
Abstract: It is proposed that real and imagined movements activate identical neural networks. Cortical oscillatory activity is proposed as a mechanism through which distributed neuronal networks may bind into coherent ensembles and coupling of oscillators is used as a tool to investigate modulations of cortical connectivity. The aim of the present study was to test the hypothesis that, although the same brain network is involved in both real and imagined movements, the functional connectivity within the network differs. To do so, we measured interregional coupling, quantified using coherence between scalp EEG electrodes, during different periods of a prehension task during real and imagined movements. The results demonstrated a different pattern of coupling in the beta frequency range between electrodes overlying occipital and motor cortices during executed and imagined movements. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the neural networks during real and imagined movements are not identical.
Notes:
2003
Fadila Hadj-Bouziane, Driss Boussaoud (2003)  Neuronal activity in the monkey striatum during conditional visuomotor learning.   Exp Brain Res 153: 2. 190-196 Nov  
Abstract: The frontostriatal system has been implicated in linking sensory information and action through arbitrary rules. The aim of the present study was to investigate the role of the striatum, a key, integrative structure in such behavior. We recorded single neuronal activity from the anterior striatum of two macaque monkeys as they learned to associate new visual cues with motor responses. Monkeys held a joystick which they could move in one of four constrained directions. During the recording sessions, they were presented with a set of well-learned associations (familiar condition), and with sets of new cues which they were required to map, by trial and error, with joystick movements (novel condition). A sample of 50 striatum neurons were studied during execution of familiar and novel conditions. Activity of most of them was strongly modulated during learning. These modulations were either transient or long lasting changes of neuronal firing rate. The findings suggest that the striatum is involved in early stages of conditional visuomotor learning, and are interpreted in relation with previous data on frontal cortex.
Notes:
Fadila Hadj-Bouziane, Martine Meunier, Driss Boussaoud (2003)  Conditional visuo-motor learning in primates: a key role for the basal ganglia.   J Physiol Paris 97: 4-6. 567-579 Jul/Nov  
Abstract: Sensory guidance of behavior often involves standard visuo-motor mapping of body movements onto objects and spatial locations. For example, looking at and reaching to grasp a glass of wine requires the mapping of the eyes and hand to the location of the glass in space, as well as the formation of a hand configuration appropriate to the shape of the glass. But our brain is far more than just a standard sensorimotor mapping machine. Through evolution, the brain of advanced mammals, in particular human and non-human primates, has acquired a formidable capacity to construct non-standard, arbitrary mapping using associations between external events and behavioral responses that bear no direct relationship. For example, we have all learned to stop at a red traffic light and to go at a green one, or to wait for a specific tone before dialing a phone number and to hang up when hearing a busy signal. These arbitrary associations are acquired through experience, thereby providing primates with a rich and flexible sensorimotor repertoire. Understanding how they are learned, and how they are recalled and used when the context requires them, has been one of the challenging issues for cognitive neuroscience. Valuable insights have been gained over the last two decades through the convergence of multiple complementary approaches. Human neuropsychology and experimental lesions in monkeys have identified a network of brain structures important for conditional sensorimotor associations, whereas imaging studies in healthy human subjects and electrophysiological recordings in awake monkeys have sought to identify the different functional processes underlying the overall function. The present review focuses on the contribution of a network linking the prefrontal cortex, basal ganglia, and dorsal premotor cortex, with special emphasis on results from recording experiments in monkeys. We will first review data pointing to a specific contribution of each component of the network to the performance of well-learned arbitrary visuo-motor associations, as well as data suggesting how novel associations are formed. Then we will propose a model positing that each component of the fronto-striatal network makes a specific contribution to the formation and/or execution of sensorimotor associations. In this model, the basal ganglia are thought to play a key role in linking the sensory, motor, and reward information necessary for arbitrary mapping.
Notes:
Driss Boussaoud (2003)  The planning of action: can one separate attention from intention?   Med Sci (Paris) 19: 5. 583-591 May  
Abstract: Attention and motor preparation are two intimately linked processes. However, they can be dissociated in the laboratory in order to study their neuronal basis. Behavioral neurophysiology has thus shown that neurons that discharge in relation with attention or with motor preparation (or intention) exist in a variety of brain regions in the monkey, especially the prefrontal and premotor cortices. When examined more carefully, these two regions appear different in both the proportion of cells that respond during attention versus intention, and in the information coded in the so-called "preparatory activity". This activity reflects sensory selection in the prefrontal cortex (spatial attention/memory), motor selection in the premotor cortex. Furthermore, two regions in the dorsal aspect of premotor cortex can be distinguished on the basis of their relative involvement in attention: a rostral (anterior) region, functionally close to prefrontal cortex, and a caudal one, which appears functionally close to motor cortex. Using an experimental design derived from monkey experiments, a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study recently indicated that the functional specialization within the premotor cortex is similar in monkey and man.
Notes:
2002
Judith TannĂ©-GariĂ©py, Driss Boussaoud, Eric M Rouiller (2002)  Projections of the claustrum to the primary motor, premotor, and prefrontal cortices in the macaque monkey.   J Comp Neurol 454: 2. 140-157 Dec  
Abstract: The claustrum is interconnected with the frontal lobe, including the motor cortex, prefrontal cortex, and cingulate cortex. The goal of the present study was to assess whether the claustral projections to distinct areas within the frontal cortex arise from separate regions within the claustrum. Multiple injections of tracers were performed in 15 macaque monkeys, aimed toward primary motor area (M1), pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA), SMA-proper, rostral (PMd-r) and caudal (PMd-c) parts of the dorsal premotor cortex (PM), rostral (PMv-r) and caudal (PMv-c) parts of the ventral PM, and superior and inferior parts of area 46. The distribution of retrogradely labeled neurons showed no clear segregation along the rostrocaudal axis of the claustrum; they were usually located along the entire anteroposterior extent of the claustrum. For all motor cortical areas, there was a general trend of the labeled neurons to occupy the dorsal and intermediate parts of the claustrum along the dorsoventral axis. The same territories were labeled after injection in area 46, but in addition numerous labeled neurons were found in the most ventral part of the claustrum. At higher resolution, however, there was clear evidence that the territories projecting to pre-SMA and SMA-proper formed separate, interdigitating, clusters along the dorsoventral axis. A comparable local segregation was observed for the two subdivisions of area 46, whereas there was more local overlap among the subareas of PM. The projections from the claustrum to the multiple subareas of the motor cortex and to area 46 arise from largely overlapping territories, with, however, some degree of local segregation.
Notes:
Judith TannĂ©-GariĂ©py, Eric M Rouiller, Driss Boussaoud (2002)  Parietal inputs to dorsal versus ventral premotor areas in the macaque monkey: evidence for largely segregated visuomotor pathways.   Exp Brain Res 145: 1. 91-103 Jul  
Abstract: The lateral premotor cortex plays a crucial role in visually guided limb movements. Visual information may reach this cortical region from the parietal cortex, the highest stage in the dorsal visual stream. Anatomical studies indicate that the parietal projections to the dorsal (PMd) and ventral (PMv) premotor areas arise from separate parietal regions, supporting the notion of parallel visuomotor pathways. We tested the degree of segregation of these pathways by injecting retrograde tracers into PMd and PMv in the same monkeys, under physiological control. Eleven injections were made in four animals, and the analysis of retrograde labelling revealed that parietal cells projecting to PMd and those projecting to PMv are largely segregated. The strongest projections to PMd arise from the superior parietal lobule, including the medial intraparietal area (MIP), PEc and PGm, and the parieto-occipital area. These areas were devoid of labelling following injections into PMv, which receives its major projections from the anterior intraparietal area (AIP), area PEip, the anterior portion of the inferior parietal gyrus (area 7b), and the somatosensory areas. In addition to their strong projections to PMv, areas 7b and PEip send minor projections to PMd as well. Additional projections to PMd arise from the ventral intraparietal area and the inferior parietal lobule. The present findings are direct anatomical evidence for largely segregated visuomotor pathways linking parietal cortex with PMd and PMv.
Notes:
Alice C Roy, Yves Paulignan, Martine Meunier, Driss Boussaoud (2002)  Prehension movements in the macaque monkey: effects of object size and location.   J Neurophysiol 88: 3. 1491-1499 Sep  
Abstract: Prehension movements were examined in freely behaving monkeys and compared with the well-known characteristics of human movements. The degree of independence of the components of movements (i.e., reaching and grasping) was investigated in animals trained to reach for and grasp three-dimensional objects. To this aim, the kinematics of prehension movements was recorded using an Optotrak system in two tasks. In one task, monkeys grasped a small or a large object (size task), in the other, they grasped an object of constant size placed at three different spatial locations (location task). We found that object size and its location affected both reaching and grasping. In particular, in the size task, we found that the maximum grip aperture strongly depended on the selection of the grip and not only on the size of an object. Our results also revealed that, in monkeys as well as in humans, the reaching parameters are highly sensitive to task-related constraints such as accuracy demands. The results of the location task showed a difference between rightward and leftward movements, a pattern of grip aperture that varied across animals, and a large degree of coordination between the two components. These findings argue against a strict postulate of independence between the visuo-motor channels, favoring instead the idea of variable degrees of coordination between the reach and grasp components depending on the task demands. Finally, this work emphasizes the relevance of studying monkey's prehension movements as a useful step to the understanding of visuo-motor control in humans.
Notes:
StĂ©phane R Simon, Martine Meunier, LoĂ¿s Piettre, Anna M Berardi, Christoph M Segebarth, Driss Boussaoud (2002)  Spatial attention and memory versus motor preparation: premotor cortex involvement as revealed by fMRI.   J Neurophysiol 88: 4. 2047-2057 Oct  
Abstract: Recent studies in both monkeys and humans indicate that the dorsal premotor cortex participates in spatial attention and working memory, in addition to its well known role in movement planning and execution. One important question is whether these functions overlap or are segregated within this frontal area. Single-cell recordings in monkeys suggest a relative specialization of the rostral portion of dorsal premotor cortex for attention and/or memory and of the caudal region for motor preparation. To test whether this possibility also holds true in humans, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to compare, in the same set of subjects, brain activation related to strong spatial attention and memory demands to that elicited by long motor preparatory periods. The behavioral protocol was based on a task that had proved effective for dissociating neuronal properties related to these two functions in the monkey brain. The principle of the monkey task was that a first cue guided the focus of spatial attention and memory, whereas a second one instructed an arm movement. Based on this principle, two tasks were developed. One maximized spatial attention and memory demands by presenting long series of stimuli (4, 8, or 12) before the motor instructional cue, whereas the other extended the motor preparation phase by imposing long and variable delays (1-5.5 s) between the onset of the instructional cue and movement execution. The two tasks and their respective control conditions were arranged in two blocked-design sequences. The results indicate that the brain networks underlying the two functional domains overlap in the caudate nucleus and presupplementary motor area, and possibly in lateral prefrontal cortex as well, but involve different dorsal premotor fields. Motor preparation primarily recruited a dorsal premotor area located caudally, within the precentral gyrus (together with the supplementary motor area), whereas spatial attention and memory preferentially activated a more rostral site, in and anterior to the precentral sulcus (in addition to the posterior parietal cortex). These findings strengthen the idea that the primate dorsal premotor cortex contributes to both motor and nonmotor processes. Moreover, they corroborate emerging evidence from monkey physiology suggesting a relative functional segregation within this cortex, with attention to short-term storage of visuospatial information engaging a more rostral region than motor preparation.
Notes:
2001
D Boussaoud (2001)  Attention versus intention in the primate premotor cortex.   Neuroimage 14: 1 Pt 2. S40-S45 Jul  
Abstract: One challenging issue in cognitive neuroscience has been to dissociate a variety of mental processes from one another in order to elucidate brain functions. Attention, in particular, has been a recurrent issue because of its strong links with perceptual, cognitive, and motor performances. This paper reviews data from neurophysiological experiments designed to dissociate neuronal activity related to visuo-spatial attention from preparatory activity in the monkey brain. Cell activity was recorded from the dorsal premotor area (PMd) and compared to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPf), from which it receives inputs. PMd has been implicated in the planing and execution of voluntary movements (PMd), and the majority of its cells are active during tasks with instructed delay time. However, the activity of many PMd cells is not specifically correlated with movement preparation, as is observed when the animal is attending to a visual stimulus, although the proportion of attention-related cells is much lower than in the DLPf. The distribution of attention-related and intention-related neurons within PMd tends to vary along the rostrocaudal axis, with the former more frequent rostrally (PMdr) and the latter more predominant caudally (PMdc). In a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study in humans, we compared the premotor activation in two tasks: a spatial attention/memory task and a motor preparation task. The results suggest a rostrocaudal specialization within PMd of the human brain, with attention-related activation rostrally and medially and intention-related activation caudally. These studies indicate strong similarities in the functional organization of dorsal premotor cortex of human and monkey.
Notes:
2000
A C Roy, Y Paulignan, A Farnè, C Jouffrais, D Boussaoud (2000)  Hand kinematics during reaching and grasping in the macaque monkey.   Behav Brain Res 117: 1-2. 75-82 Dec  
Abstract: In this paper, we develop an animal model of prehension movements by examining the kinematics of reaching and grasping in monkeys and by comparing the results to published data on humans. Hand movements were recorded in three dimensions in monkeys who were trained to either point at visual targets under unperturbed and perturbed conditions, or to reach and grasp 3-D objects. The results revealed the following three similarities in the hand kinematics of monkey and man. (1) Pointing movements showed an asymmetry depending on target location relative to the hand used; in particular, movements to an ipsilateral target took longer than those to a contralateral one. (2) Perturbation of target location decreased the magnitude of the velocity peak and increased the duration of pointing movements. (3) Reaching to grasp movements displayed a bell-shaped wrist velocity profile and the maximum grip aperture was correlated with object size. These similarities indicate that the macaque monkey can be a useful model for understanding human motor control.
Notes:
D M Clower, D Boussaoud (2000)  Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition.   J Neurophysiol 84: 5. 2703-2708 Nov  
Abstract: Exposure to laterally displacing prisms is characterized by systematic misreaching in the opposite direction after prisms are removed. Other learning tasks involving altered visuomotor mappings can often be mastered by the subject with minimal resulting aftereffects. One variable that may account for this difference is the nature of the feedback provided to the subject: during studies of prism exposure, subjects usually view the hand itself, whereas in many studies of visuomotor learning, subjects view a computer-generated representation of the hand position or movement. We compared the use of actual feedback of the hand with computer-generated representational feedback of its position during exposure to laterally displacing prisms. In the actual feedback condition (ACT), a light on the fingertip was illuminated immediately at the end of each reach. In the representational feedback condition (REP), a computer-generated spot of light was displayed to indicate the exact position of the fingertip at the end of each reach. Whereas the rate and magnitude of error correction were the same in both conditions, only the ACT condition produced the large adaptive aftereffect typically observed after prism exposure. These results suggest that the perception of a physical coincidence between the feedback source and the hand may be a key factor in determining whether adaptation is accomplished through perceptual recalibration or visuomotor skill acquisition.
Notes:
1999
D Boussaoud, F Bremmer (1999)  Gaze effects in the cerebral cortex: reference frames for space coding and action.   Exp Brain Res 128: 1-2. 170-180 Sep  
Abstract: Visual information is mapped with respect to the retina within the early stages of the visual cortex. On the other hand, the brain has to achieve a representation of object location in a coordinate system that matches the reference frame used by the motor cortex to code reaching movement in space. The mechanism of the necessary coordinate transformation between the different frames of reference from the visual to the motor system as well as its localization within the cerebral cortex is still unclear. Coordinate transformation is traditionally described as a series of elementary computations along the visuomotor cortical pathways, and the motor system is thought to receive target information in a body-centered reference frame. However, neurons along these pathways have a number of similar properties and receive common input signals, suggesting that a non-retinocentric representation of object location in space might be available for sensory and motor purposes throughout the visuomotor pathway. This paper reviews recent findings showing that elementary input signals, such as retinal and eye position signals, reach the dorsal premotor cortex. We will also compare eye position effects in the premotor cortex with those described in the posterior parietal cortex. Our main thesis is that appropriate sensory input signals are distributed across the visuomotor continuum, and could potentially allow, in parallel, the emergence of multiple and task-dependent reference frames.
Notes:
E M Rouiller, J Tanne, V Moret, D Boussaoud (1999)  Origin of thalamic inputs to the primary, premotor, and supplementary motor cortical areas and to area 46 in macaque monkeys: a multiple retrograde tracing study.   J Comp Neurol 409: 1. 131-152 Jun  
Abstract: The origin of thalamic inputs to distinct motor cortical areas was established in five monkeys to determine whether the motor areas receive inputs from a common thalamic nucleus and the extent to which the territories of origin overlap. To not rely on the rough definition of cytoarchitectonic boundaries in the thalamus, monkeys were subjected to multiple injections of tracers (four to seven) in the primary (M1), premotor (PM), and supplementary (SMA) motor cortical areas and in area 46. The cortical areas were distributed into five groups, each receiving inputs from a specific set of thalamic nuclei: 1) M1; 2) SMA-proper and the caudal part of the dorsal PM (PMdc); 3) the rostral and caudal parts of the ventral PM (PMvr and PMvc); 4) the rostral part of the dorsal PM (PMdr); and 5) the superior and inferior parts of area 46 (area 46sup and area 46inf). A major degree of overlap was obtained for the origins of the thalamocortical projections directed to areas 46inf and 46sup and for those terminating in SMA-proper and PMdc. PMvc and PMvr received inputs from adjacent and/or common thalamic regions. In contrast, the degree of overlap between M1 and SMA was smaller. The projection to M1 shared relatively limited zones of origin with the projections directed to PM. Thalamic inputs to the motor cortical areas (M1, SMA, PMd, and PMv), in general, were segregated from those directed to area 46, except in the mediodorsal nucleus, in which there was clear overlap of the territories sending projections to area 46, SMA-proper, and PMdc.
Notes:
C Jouffrais, D Boussaoud (1999)  Neuronal activity related to eye-hand coordination in the primate premotor cortex.   Exp Brain Res 128: 1-2. 205-209 Sep  
Abstract: To test the functional implications of gaze signals that we previously reported in the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), we trained two rhesus monkeys to point to visual targets presented on a touch screen while controlling their gaze orientation. Each monkey had to perform four different tasks. To initiate a trial, the monkey had to put his hand on a starting position at the center of the touch screen and fixate a fixation point. In one task, the animal had to make a reaching movement to a peripheral target randomly presented at one of eight possible locations on a circle while maintaining fixation at the center of this virtual circle (central fixation + reaching). In the second task, the monkey maintained fixation at the location of the upcoming peripheral target and, later, reached to that location. After a delay, the target was turned on and the monkey made a reaching arm movement (target fixation + reaching). In the third task, the monkey made a saccade to the target without any arm movement (saccade). Finally, in the fourth task, the monkey first made a saccade to the target, then reached to it after a delay (saccade + reaching). This design allowed us to examine the contribution of the oculomotor context to arm-related neuronal activity in PMd. We analyzed the effects of the task type on neuronal activity and found that many cells showed a task effect during the signal (26/60; 43%), set (16/49; 33%) and/or movement (15/54; 28%) epochs, depending on the oculomotor history. These findings, together with previously published data, suggest that PMd codes limb-movement direction in a gaze-dependent manner and may, thus, play an important role in the brain mechanisms of eye-hand coordination during visually guided reaching.
Notes:
1998
E M Rouiller, J TannĂ©, V Moret, I Kermadi, D Boussaoud, E Welker (1998)  Dual morphology and topography of the corticothalamic terminals originating from the primary, supplementary motor, and dorsal premotor cortical areas in macaque monkeys.   J Comp Neurol 396: 2. 169-185 Jun  
Abstract: In the motor, somatosensory, and auditory systems of rodents and cats, the corticothalamic connection is composed of a main projection formed by small endings and a minor projection terminating with giant endings. To establish whether the corticothalamic projection originating from motor cortical areas in primates exhibits the same duality, the anterograde tracer biotinylated dextran amine was injected in eight macaque monkeys in the primary motor (M1; n = 3), the supplementary motor (SMA; n = 3) and the dorsal premotor (PMd; n = 2) cortical areas to label corticothalamic axons. The corticothalamic projection originating from these three motor cortical areas was characterized by the presence of axon terminals constituting the same two types of endings, observed both as boutons en passant and terminaux. The population of small endings exhibited a mean cross-sectional maximum diameter of 0.95 microm (S.D. = 0.23), a range of diameters not overlapping that of giant endings (mean diameter = 3.46 microm, S.D. = 0.74 microm). Topographically, the giant endings originating from M1 were located in the same thalamic nucleus (ventroposterolateral nucleus, oral part) in which the small endings were found. In contrast, the giant endings originating from SMA and PMd were located in a thalamic nucleus (mediodorsal nucleus) distinct from the main termination zone formed by small endings. Along the rostrocaudal axis, the giant endings were distributed in a restricted zone, irrespective of the origin of the projection (M1, SMA, PMd). The dual morphology of corticothalamic endings, previously found in rodents and cats, is present in the motor system of subhuman primates for both primary and nonprimary motor cortical areas.
Notes:
D Boussaoud, C Jouffrais, F Bremmer (1998)  Eye position effects on the neuronal activity of dorsal premotor cortex in the macaque monkey.   J Neurophysiol 80: 3. 1132-1150 Sep  
Abstract: Visual inputs to the brain are mapped in a retinocentric reference frame, but the motor system plans movements in a body-centered frame. This basic observation implies that the brain must transform target coordinates from one reference frame to another. Physiological studies revealed that the posterior parietal cortex may contribute a large part of such a transformation, but the question remains as to whether the premotor areas receive visual information, from the parietal cortex, readily coded in body-centered coordinates. To answer this question, we studied dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) neurons in two monkeys while they performed a conditional visuomotor task and maintained fixation at different gaze angles. Visual stimuli were presented on a video monitor, and the monkeys made limb movements on a panel of three touch pads located at the bottom of the monitor. A trial begins when the monkey puts its hand on the central pad. Then, later in the trial, a colored cue instructed a limb movement to the left touch pad if red or to the right one if green. The cues lasted for a variable delay, the instructed delay period, and their offset served as the go signal. The fixation spot was presented at the center of the screen or at one of four peripheral locations. Because the monkey's head was restrained, peripheral fixations caused a deviation of the eyes within the orbit, but for each fixation angle, the instructional cue was presented at nine locations with constant retinocentric coordinates. After the presentation of the instructional cue, 133 PMd cells displayed a phasic discharge (signal-related activity), 157 were tonically active during the instructed delay period (set-related or preparatory activity), and 104 were active after the go signal in relation to movement (movement-related activity). A large proportion of cells showed variations of the discharge rate in relation to limb movement direction, but only modest proportions were sensitive to the cue's location (signal, 43%; set, 34%; movement, 29%). More importantly, the activity of most neurons (signal, 74%; set, 79%; movement, 79%) varied significantly (analysis of variance, P < 0.05) with orbital eye position. A regression analysis showed that the neuronal activity varied linearly with eye position along the horizontal and vertical axes and can be approximated by a two-dimensional regression plane. These data provide evidence that eye position signals modulate the neuronal activity beyond sensory areas, including those involved in visually guided reaching limb movements. Further, they show that neuronal activity related to movement preparation and execution combines at least two directional parameters: arm movement direction and gaze direction in space. It is suggested that a substantial population of PMd cells codes limb movement direction in a head-centered reference frame.
Notes:
1997
P F Dominey, D Boussaoud (1997)  Encoding behavioral context in recurrent networks of the fronto-striatal system: a simulation study.   Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 6: 1. 53-65 Jul  
Abstract: This research addresses the hypothesis that behavioral context is encoded in recurrent networks of the fronto-striatal system. Behavioral context influences the processing of subsequent brain events, including responses to sensory inputs, thus providing a basis for context-dependent behavior. We define context-dependent behavior as the adaptive ability to produce the appropriate response to a given stimulus, dependent upon the context in which it appears. Behavioral context can change with a time-scale on the order of seconds to tens of seconds or more. This suggests a flexible mechanism that encodes context via an ensemble of neural activation that will appropriately influence the processing of subsequent sensory stimuli. We present a functional model of context encoding in recurrent connections of the fronto-striatal system with simulation results that correspond closely to empirical data. Neuronal activity in monkeys that perform a context-dependent task indicate that the prefrontal cortex and striatum participate differentially in this kind of context encoding. Likewise, simulated neurons in our model of the fronto-striatal system, which performs the context-dependent task, display task-related activity remarkably similar to that found in monkey frontal cortex and striatum, supporting our hypothesis.
Notes:
D Boussaoud, I Kermadi (1997)  The primate striatum: neuronal activity in relation to spatial attention versus motor preparation.   Eur J Neurosci 9: 10. 2152-2168 Oct  
Abstract: The primate basal ganglia are known to be involved in the initiation and control of visually guided movements. However, the precise role of these structures is not clear, partly because most neurophysiological studies have not dissociated neuronal activity related to visuomotor processing from that reflecting other aspects of behaviour, such as shifts of spatial attention. Moreover, the way the basal ganglia function together with the frontal cortex during movement initiation and execution is still a matter of debate. In an effort to clarify these issues, we recorded single neurons from the striatum (caudate nucleus and putamen) in two rhesus monkeys trained to perform a conditional visuomotor task, and compared their properties with those of the frontal cortex. The experimental paradigm was designed to distinguish neuronal activity associated with shifts of attention from that reflecting motor preparation. In a given trial, an identical visual stimulus could serve as a cue for the reorientation of spatial attention or as a cue for establishing a motor set depending on when it occurred during that trial. Additional aspects of the paradigm were designed to identify neurons whose activity differed when various stimulus configurations instructed the same action (stimulus effect), as well as neurons whose activity differed when two different actions were instructed by the same stimulus (movement effect). The majority of cells (60%) were preferentially active after instructional cues, 38% discharged preferentially after attentional cues, and the remaining 2% of cells discharged equally after both types of cue. Neurons active after instructional cues were further analysed for stimulus and movement effects. During movement preparation, the activity of the vast majority of striatal cells (putamen, 81%; caudate, 76%) varied significantly when different stimuli instructed the same action. Likewise, when different movements were instructed by the same stimulus, preparatory activity of a majority of cells (putamen, 92%; caudate, 82%) changed. Consequently, a substantial proportion of cells showed combined stimulus and movement effects. Comparison of these neuronal properties with those of the dorsal premotor cortex showed significantly higher proportions of cells in the striatum whose activity reflected sensory or sensorimotor processing. These results suggest that the basal ganglia are involved in shifting attentional set and in high-order processes of movement initiation, including the linking of sensory information with behavioural responses.
Notes:
S P Wise, D Boussaoud, P B Johnson, R Caminiti (1997)  Premotor and parietal cortex: corticocortical connectivity and combinatorial computations.   Annu Rev Neurosci 20: 25-42  
Abstract: The dorsal premotor cortex is a functionally distinct cortical field or group of fields in the primate frontal cortex. Anatomical studies have confirmed that most parietal input to the dorsal premotor cortex originates from the superior parietal lobule. However, these projections arise not only from the dorsal aspect of area 5, as has long been known, but also from newly defined areas of posterior parietal cortex, which are directly connected with the extrastriate visual cortex. Thus, the dorsal premotor cortex receives much more direct visual input than previously accepted. It appears that this fronto-parietal network functions as a visuomotor controller-one that makes computations based on proprioceptive, visual, gaze, attentional, and other information to produce an output that reflects the selection, preparation, and execution of movements.
Notes:
1996
E M Rouiller, V Moret, J Tanne, D Boussaoud (1996)  Evidence for direct connections between the hand region of the supplementary motor area and cervical motoneurons in the macaque monkey.   Eur J Neurosci 8: 5. 1055-1059 May  
Abstract: In primates the corticospinal neurons of the hand representation of the primary motor cortex (M1) give rise to direct contacts with the cervical motoneurons that control distal forelimb muscles. We investigated, at the light-microscopy level, whether corticospinal cells present in the hand area of the supplementary motor area (SMA) also establish direct connections with cervical motoneurons, particularly those innervating hand and finger muscles. The hand representation of the M1 (two monkeys) or SMA (two monkeys) was located using intracortical microstimulation and injected with the anterograde tracer biotinylated dextran amine to label corticospinal terminals. Forearm muscles acting on the wrist and hand as well as hand muscles acting on the thumb and index finger, thus including those activated by intracortical stimulation, were injected with the retrograde tracer cholera-toxin B subunit, in order to label the motoneurons. A consistent zone of overlap between the two markers was found in the cervical cord. Close appositions between corticospinal axonal terminals and the somata or dendrites of motoneurons were found after injection in the M1, confirming previous observations. The new finding is the observation of similar close appositions after injection in the SMA, suggesting its control of hand movements in parallel with the M1.
Notes:
S P Wise, G di Pellegrino, D Boussaoud (1996)  The premotor cortex and nonstandard sensorimotor mapping.   Can J Physiol Pharmacol 74: 4. 469-482 Apr  
Abstract: We often gaze at and attend to an object while preparing to reach toward and grasp it, and continue doing so when the plan is executed. Elaborate machinery, much of it in the brainstem and spinal cord, provides control systems for the spatially congruent guidance of the eyes, limbs, and body toward targets in visual space. We will use the term standard mapping for the sensorimotor transformations that underlie such behavior. Despite the common sense character of standard mapping, the targets of gaze, attention, and reaching can be dissociated from each other. We can attend to stimuli in locations that differ from the target of action. We can gaze in one direction while reaching in another. And we can guide spatial action with nonspatial stimuli, such as when, in conditional motor tasks, the color of an object instructs a movement elsewhere in space. All of these situations, and many others, call for a process that we term nonstandard mapping, wherein the central nervous system must reject the commonplace correspondences among visuospatial stimuli, gaze, attention, and reaching movements. We focus in this article on the possibility that premotor cortex underlies nonstandard mapping and, therefore, the behavioral flexibility that such a process allows.
Notes:
1995
D Boussaoud, G di Pellegrino, S P Wise (1995)  Frontal lobe mechanisms subserving vision-for-action versus vision-for-perception.   Behav Brain Res 72: 1-2. 1-15 Dec  
Abstract: In the typical course of daily events, we often gaze at an object, attend to its features and its place, reach toward it and grasp it, all with an awareness of what we are doing at the time. But behavior is not always thus. Gaze, attention, limb movement direction and awareness can be behaviorally dissociated from each other, and this review focuses on one such dissociation: that between the perception of an object and the use of that object's inherent spatial and nonspatial information for mediating visuomotor control. We review evidence that partially different neuronal systems underlie these two aspects of visual information processing. In neurophysiological studies of the primate frontal lobe, it has been possible to demonstrate that neural signals appearing to be visual responses reflect, at least in part, the motor significance of a stimulus. This finding has been confirmed, in separate studies, for both spatial and nonspatial visual information and supports the hypothesis that some frontal cortex activity reflects the selection and guidance of action rather than the properties of visual stimuli, per se. These findings are discussed in the context of neuropsychological studies indicating that accurate and appropriate movements are possible without perceptual awareness of the information guiding those movements.
Notes:
I Kermadi, D Boussaoud (1995)  Role of the primate striatum in attention and sensorimotor processes: comparison with premotor cortex.   Neuroreport 6: 8. 1177-1181 May  
Abstract: The present study was aimed at distinguishing neuronal activity associated with shifts of attention from that reflecting motor set. Our behavioural paradigm allowed a given visual stimulus to serve as a cue for the reorientation of spatial attention or as a cue for establishing a motor set, depending on when it occurred during a trial. Other aspects of the paradigm were designed to identify neurones whose activity differed when various stimulus configurations instructed the same action, as well as neurones whose activity differed when two different limb movements were instructed by the same stimulus. We found that many striatal cells discharge preferentially in relation to cues which reorient spatial attention, although they may also discharge after cues which instruct a motor act. In contrast to the dorsal premotor area (PMd, dorsolateral area 6), in both the caudate nucleus and putamen a larger proportion of the neuronal sample reflected both movement direction and stimulus attributes. These results support a role for striatal neurones in both attentional set shifting and the preparation for context-specific actions.
Notes:
D Boussaoud (1995)  Primate premotor cortex: modulation of preparatory neuronal activity by gaze angle.   J Neurophysiol 73: 2. 886-890 Feb  
Abstract: 1. This study investigated whether the neuronal activity of a cortical area devoted to the control of limb movements is affected by variations in eye position within the orbit. Two rhesus monkeys were trained to perform a conditional visuomotor task with an instructed delay period while maintaining gaze on a fixation point. 2. The experimental design required each monkey to put its hand on a metal touch pad located at arm's length and fixate a small spot of light presented on a computer screen. Then a visual cue came on, at the fixation point or elsewhere, the color of which instructed the monkey to move its limb to one of two touch pads according to a conditional rule. A red cue meant a movement to the left, whereas a green one instructed a movement to the right. The cue lasted for a variable delay period (1-3 s), and the monkey had to wait for its offset, the go signal, before performing the correct response. The fixation point and the cues were presented at various screen locations in a combination that allowed examination of whether eye position and/or target position modulate the neuronal activity. Because the monkeys' heads were fixed, all changes in eye position reflected movements in a craniocentric, head-centered, coordinate space. 3. The activity of single neurons was recorded from dorsal premotor cortex (PMd). For most neurons (79%), the activity during the instructed delay period (set-related activity) reflects the direction of the upcoming limb movement but varies significantly with eye position.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)
Notes:
J TannĂ©, D Boussaoud, N Boyer-Zeller, E M Rouiller (1995)  Direct visual pathways for reaching movements in the macaque monkey.   Neuroreport 7: 1. 267-272 Dec  
Abstract: The brain seems to process the location of objects faster than their intrinsic features, such as size, when these parameters are used to guide action. To uncover a potential anatomical substrate of these different processing speeds, we investigated in the monkey the pathways linking extrastriate visual cortex with the dorsal premotor area, a frontal area known to be involved in visually guided reaching movements. Retrogradely transported anatomical tracers were injected at physiologically defined sites and the distribution of labelled cells was examined in the ipsilateral cortex. We found a projection to the dorsal premotor cortex from the parieto-occipital area (PO). This area receives direct projections from the primary visual cortex (V1), and is part of the dorsal visual stream involved in the processing of spatial information. No direct projections to the dorsal premotor cortex arise from the ventral visual areas, thought to process object features. Our finding provides evidence for direct pathways from the dorsal visual stream to the dorsal premotor cortex and supports the view that the location of objects is processed faster by the brain than their intrinsic features.
Notes:
1993
D Boussaoud, S P Wise (1993)  Primate frontal cortex: neuronal activity following attentional versus intentional cues.   Exp Brain Res 95: 1. 15-27  
Abstract: We examined neuronal activity in three parts of the primate frontal cortex: the dorsal (PMd) and ventral (PMv) premotor cortex and a ventrolateral part of the dorsolateral prefrontal (PF) cortex. Two monkeys fixated a 0.2 degrees white square in the center of a video display while depressing a switch located between two touch pads. On each trial, a spatial-attentional/mnemonic (SAM) cue was presented first. The SAM cue consisted of one 2 degrees x 2 degrees square, usually red or green, and its location indicated where a conditional motor instruction would appear after a delay period. The stimulus event containing the motor instruction, termed the motor instructional/conditional (MIC) cue, could be of two general types. It might consist of a single 2 degrees x 2 degrees square stimulus identical to one of the SAM cues presented at the same location as the SAM cue on that trial. When the MIC cue was a single square, it instructed the monkey to move its forelimb to one of the two touch pads according to the following conditional rule: a green MIC cue meant that contact with the right touch pad would be rewarded on that trial and a red MIC cue instructed a movement to the left touch pad. Alternatively, the MIC cue might consist of two 2 degrees x 2 degrees squares, only one of which was at the SAM-cue location: in those cases, one square was red and the other was green. The colored square at the SAM cue location for that trial was the instructing stimulus, and the other part of the MIC cue was irrelevant. When, after a variable delay period, the MIC cue disappeared, the monkey had to touch the appropriate target within 1 s to receive a reward and could break visual fixation. The experimental design allowed comparison of frontal cortical activity when one stimulus, identical in retinocentric, craniocentric, and allocentric spatial location as well as all other stimulus parameters, had two different meanings for the animal's behavior. When a stimulus was the SAM cue, it led to either a reorientation of spatial attention to its location, or the storage of its location in spatial memory. By contrast, when it was the MIC cue, the same stimulus instructed a motor act to be executed after a delay period. For the majority of PMd neurons (55%), post-MIC cue activity exceeded post-SAM cue activity.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
Notes:
C Distler, D Boussaoud, R Desimone, L G Ungerleider (1993)  Cortical connections of inferior temporal area TEO in macaque monkeys.   J Comp Neurol 334: 1. 125-150 Aug  
Abstract: In macaque monkeys, lesions involving the posterior portion of the inferior temporal cortex, cytoarchitectonic area TEO, produce a severe impairment in visual pattern discrimination. Recently, this area has been shown to contain a complete, though coarse, representation of the contralateral visual field (Boussaoud, Desimone, and Ungerleider: J. Comp. Neurol. 306:554-575, '91). Because the inputs and outputs of area TEO have not yet been fully described, we injected a variety of retrograde and anterograde tracers into 11 physiologically identified sites within TEO of seven rhesus monkeys and analyzed the areal and laminar distribution of its cortical connections. Our results show that TEO receives feedforward, topographically organized inputs from prestriate areas V2, V3, and V4. Additional sparser feedforward inputs arise from areas V3A, V4t, and MT. Each of these inputs is reciprocated by a feedback projection from TEO. TEO was also found to have reciprocal intermediate-type connections with the fundus of the superior temporal area (area FST), cortex in the most posteromedial portion of the superior temporal sulcus (the posterior parietal sulcal zone [area PP]), cortex in the intraparietal sulcus (including the lateral intraparietal area [area LIP]), the frontal eye field, and area TF on the parahippocampal gyrus. The connections with V3A, V4t, and PP were found only after injections in the peripheral field representations of TEO. Finally, TEO was found to project in a feedforward pattern to area TE and to areas anterior to FST on the lateral bank and floor of the superior temporal sulcus (areas TEm, TEa, and IPa, Seltzer and Pandya: Brain Res. 149:1-24, '78), all of which send feedback projections to TEO. Feedback projections also arise from parahippocampal area TH, and areas TG, 36, and possibly 35. These are complemented by only sparse feedforward projections to TG from central field representations in TEO and to TH from peripheral field representations. The results thus indicate that TEO forms an important link in the occipitotemporal pathway for object recognition, sending visual information forward from V1 and prestriate relays in V2-V4 to anterior inferior temporal area TE.
Notes:
D Boussaoud, T M Barth, S P Wise (1993)  Effects of gaze on apparent visual responses of frontal cortex neurons.   Exp Brain Res 93: 3. 423-434  
Abstract: Previous reports have argued that single neurons in the ventral premotor cortex of rhesus monkeys (PMv, the ventrolateral part of Brodmann's area 6) typically show spatial response fields that are independent of gaze angle. We reinvestigated this issue for PMv and also explored the adjacent prearcuate cortex (PAv, areas 12 and 45). Two rhesus monkeys were operantly conditioned to press a switch and maintain fixation on a small visual stimulus (0.2 degree x 0.2 degree) while a second visual stimulus (1 degree x 1 degree or 2 degrees x 2 degrees) appeared at one of several possible locations on a video screen. When the second stimulus dimmed, after an unpredictable period of 0.4-1.2 s, the monkey had to quickly release the switch to receive liquid reinforcement. By presenting stimuli at fixed screen locations and varying the location of the fixation point, we could determine whether single neurons encode stimulus location in "absolute space" or any other coordinate system independent of gaze. For the vast majority of neurons in both PMv (90%) and PAv (94%), the apparent response to a stimulus at a given screen location varied significantly and dramatically with gaze angle. Thus, we found little evidence for gaze-independent activity in either PMv or PAv neurons. The present result in frontal cortex resembles that in posterior parietal cortex, where both retinal image location and eye position affect responsiveness to visual stimuli.
Notes:
D Boussaoud, S P Wise (1993)  Primate frontal cortex: effects of stimulus and movement.   Exp Brain Res 95: 1. 28-40  
Abstract: We compared neuronal activity in the dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), ventral premotor cortex (PMv), and prefrontal (PF) cortex of two rhesus monkeys. The behavioral design was a variant of the instructed delay task which established that: (1) a given visual stimulus could, on different trials, instruct different limb movements and (2) several different visual stimuli could instruct the same movement. Neurons in all frontal areas displayed the often replicated activity patterns that occur during instructed delay tasks, including phasic increases after instruction stimuli (signal-related activity), tonic discharge during an instructed delay period (set-related activity), and phasic premovement discharge (movement-related activity). For signal-, set-, and movement-related activity, the majority of neurons in PMd (51-64%), but only a minority in PF (16-18%) and PMv (32-40%), showed activity levels that significantly depended on the action instructed by that stimulus rather than simply the characteristics of the stimulus per se. Thus, most PMd activity, including the aspects that most resembled a sensory response, reflected factors in addition to the signal. Taken together with the results of related studies, it seems most likely that these other factors are dominated by the motor instructional significance of the stimulus. In addition, many neurons (17-37%) in all examined areas showed activity that significantly depended on which of various stimuli guided the same movement. This finding shows that, in those frontal areas, neuronal activity can be affected by both the action to be taken and the events guiding that action.
Notes:
1992
S P Wise, G Di Pellegrino, D Boussaoud (1992)  Primate premotor cortex: dissociation of visuomotor from sensory signals.   J Neurophysiol 68: 3. 969-972 Sep  
Abstract: 1. If we assume adequate control for attention, memory, and stimulus location, a bona fide sensory response would be unaffected by whether a visuospatial stimulus instructs (1) one limb movement versus another or (2) limb movement versus a shift in spatial attention or memory. Two behavioral methods tested whether apparently sensory responses in the monkey's premotor cortex are strictly that, or, alternatively, whether they reflect the action instructed by a stimulus. 2. When an identical stimulus leads to two different responses, phasic discharge after a visuospatial stimulus is significantly, often dramatically, affected by the response. Similarly, premotor cortex neurons discharge more after a stimulus instructs a limb movement than after the same stimulus instructs a shift in spatial attention or memory. Thus, for the majority of premotor cortex neurons, the hypothesis that phasic poststimulus activity modulation represents a sensory response can be rejected.
Notes:
D Boussaoud, R Desimone, L G Ungerleider (1992)  Subcortical connections of visual areas MST and FST in macaques.   Vis Neurosci 9: 3-4. 291-302 Sep/Oct  
Abstract: To examine the subcortical connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas (MST and FST, respectively), we injected anterograde and retrograde tracers into 16 physiologically identified sites within the two areas in seven macaque monkeys. The subcortical connections of MST and FST were found to be very similar. Both areas were found to be reciprocally connected with the pulvinar, mainly with its medial subdivision, and with the claustrum. Nonreciprocal projections from both MST and FST were consistently found in the striatum (caudate and putamen), reticular nucleus of the thalamus, and the pontine nuclei. The labeled terminals in the pons were in the dorsolateral, lateral, dorsal, and peduncular nuclei. Additional nonreciprocal projections were found in one MST and one FST case to the nucleus of the optic tract, and, in one FST case, to the lateral terminal nucleus. Finally, three cases showed a nonreciprocal projection to FST from the basal forebrain. The subcortical structures containing label following MST and FST injections were largely the same as those labeled after injections of the middle temporal visual area (MT), but the label within each structure after MST and FST injections was more widespread than that from MT, overlapping the distribution of label that has been reported after injections of parietal visual areas. This finding is consistent with the known contributions of MST and FST to the functions of parietal cortex, such as eye-movement control.
Notes:
1991
D Boussaoud, R Desimone, L G Ungerleider (1991)  Visual topography of area TEO in the macaque.   J Comp Neurol 306: 4. 554-575 Apr  
Abstract: Previous studies have mapped the visuotopic organization of visual areas from V1 through V4 in the occipital cortex and of area TE in the temporal cortex, but the cortex in between, at the occipito-temporal junction, has remained relatively unexplored. To determine the visuotopic organization of this region, receptive fields were mapped at 1,200 visually responsive sites on 370 penetrations in the ventral occipital and temporal cortex of five macaques. We identified a new visual area, roughly corresponding to cytoarchitectonic area TEO, located between the ventral portion of V4 and area TE. Receptive fields in TEO are intermediate in size between those in V4 and TE and have a coarse visuotopic organization. Collectively, receptive fields in TEO appear to cover nearly the entire contralateral visual field. The foveal and parafoveal representation of TEO is located laterally on the convexity of the inferior temporal gyrus, and the peripheral field is represented medially on the ventral surface of the hemisphere, within and medial to the occipitotemporal sulcus. Beyond the medial border of TEO, within cyteoarchitectonic area TF, is another visually responsive region, which we have termed VTF; this region may also have some crude visual topography. Bands of constant eccentricity in TEO appear to be continuous with those in V2, V3v, and V4. The upper field representation in TEO is located adjacent to that in ventral V4, with a representation of the horizontal meridian forming the boundary between the two areas. The lower field representation in TEO is located just anterior to the upper field but is smaller. In contrast to the orderly representation of eccentricity in TEO, we found little consistent representation of polar angle, other than the separation of upper and lower fields. The results of injecting anatomical tracers in two animals suggest that TEO is an important link in the pathway that relays visual information from V1 to the inferior temporal cortex. TEO is thus likely to play an important role in pattern perception.
Notes:
1990
D Boussaoud, L G Ungerleider, R Desimone (1990)  Pathways for motion analysis: cortical connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque.   J Comp Neurol 296: 3. 462-495 Jun  
Abstract: To identify the cortical connections of the medial superior temporal (MST) and fundus of the superior temporal (FST) visual areas in the extrastriate cortex of the macaque, we injected multiple tracers, both anterograde and retrograde, in each of seven macaques under physiological control. We found that, in addition to connections with each other, both MST and FST have widespread connections with visual and polysensory areas in posterior prestriate, parietal, temporal, and frontal cortex. In prestriate cortex, both areas have connections with area V3A. MST alone has connections with the far peripheral field representations of V1 and V2, the parieto-occipital (PO) visual area, and the dorsal prelunate area (DP), whereas FST alone has connections with area V4 and the dorsal portion of area V3. Within the caudal superior temporal sulcus, both areas have extensive connections with the middle temporal area (MT), MST alone has connections with area PP, and FST alone has connections with area V4t. In the rostral superior temporal sulcus, both areas have extensive connections with the superior temporal polysensory area (STP) in the upper bank of the sulcus and with area IPa in the sulcal floor. FST also has connections with the cortex in the lower bank of the sulcus, involving area TEa. In the parietal cortex, both the central field representation of MST and FST have connections with the ventral intraparietal (VIP) and lateral intraparietal (LIP) areas, whereas MST alone has connections with the inferior parietal gyrus. In the temporal cortex, the central field representation of MST as well as FST has connections with visual area TEO and cytoarchitectonic area TF. In the frontal cortex, both MST and FST have connections with the frontal eye field. On the basis of the laminar pattern of anterograde and retrograde label, it was possible to classify connections as forward, backward, or intermediate and thereby place visual areas into a cortical hierarchy. In general, MST and FST receive forward inputs from prestriate visual areas, have intermediate connections with parietal areas, and project forward to the frontal eye field and areas in the rostral superior temporal sulcus. Because of the strong inputs to MST and FST from area MT, an area known to play a role in the analysis of visual motion, and because MST and FST themselves have high proportions of directionally selective cells, they appear to be important stations in a cortical motion processing system.
Notes:
1985
J P Joseph, D Boussaoud, B Biguer (1985)  Activity of neurons in the cat substantia nigra pars reticulata during drinking.   Exp Brain Res 60: 2. 375-379  
Abstract: Extracellular activity of single neurons in the pars reticulata of the Substantia Nigra (SNpr) was recorded in cats during drinking. Two groups of cells were distinguished: I. Somatosensory cells which responded by a short decrease in firing rate to the arrival of water against the upper lip. We suggest that these stimulus-related responses reflect a complex process linked to preparation of buccolingual movements. II. Action-related cells which were de-activated at the beginning or during the entire drinking period but without modulation in relation to the individual movements of jaws and tongue. We suggest that de-activation of these cells during drinking operates as a gating mechanism which allows implementation of complex motor sequences by cortical and/or subcortical structures.
Notes:
J P Joseph, D Boussaoud (1985)  Role of the cat substantia nigra pars reticulata in eye and head movements. I. Neural activity.   Exp Brain Res 57: 2. 286-296  
Abstract: Single unit activity was recorded in the Substantia Nigra pars reticulata (SNpr) of cats trained to orient their gaze toward visual and/or auditory targets. Cells in the SNpr have a steady high rate of spontaneous activity ranging from 35 to 120 spikes per second. The neurons respond to sensory stimuli or in relation to saccadic eye movements with a decrease or a cut-off of the spontaneous discharge. Among 109 cells recorded in the SNPR 60 were responsive to visual stimuli (mean latency = 118 ms). Most of the receptive fields which were plotted were large encompassing part of the ipsilateral field. Thirty nine (39) cells were responsive to auditory stimuli (mean latency = 81 ms). A majority of these cells showed a better response for stimuli located in the contralateral hemifield. In a few cells, the sensory responses were modulated by the subsequent orienting behavior of the animals. Thirty one (31) cells showed a response in relation to saccades. These units typically stopped discharging between 50 and 300 ms prior to the onset of the saccade. 39% of these units also responded in relation to spontaneous saccades in the dark. 61% of the saccadic cells also responded to sensory stimuli in the absence of saccades. Six (6) cells were found to respond to active head movements. These results are discussed in the framework of the role that the basal ganglia might have in the selection of the sensory stimuli that trigger orienting behaviors.
Notes:
D Boussaoud, J P Joseph (1985)  Role of the cat substantia nigra pars reticulata in eye and head movements. II. Effects of local pharmacological injections.   Exp Brain Res 57: 2. 297-304  
Abstract: Guided and reflex eye movements were studied in cats trained to make orienting saccades toward visual and auditory targets. Injections of a GABA-agonist (Muscimol) or GABA-antagonists (Bicuculline and Picrotoxin) were made in the Substantia Nigra pars reticulata (SNpr). Bicuculline and Picrotoxin, whether unilaterally or bilaterally injected had no effect on the posture nor the oculomotor performance of the animals. Neck muscle activity remained symmetrical. Unilateral injections of Muscimol produced oro-facial akinesia, reduction of the number of eye movements, contralateral head turning, visual neglect mostly (but not only) for ipsilateral visual space. Balance between the gains of the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) in the two directions of movement was changed. Gain was decreased for the ipsilateral rotation. The optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) was not affected. Contralateral neck muscles were hypertonic. After bilateral injections of Muscimol, the cats did not orient. The VOR was normal when the injections induced no postural asymmetry. Hypertony was bilateral. Implications of these results for the role of the basal ganglia in motor control are discussed. We suggest that in Parkinson's disease the fixed inhibitory drive of the SNpr on the tectum and on the thalamus is disrupted.
Notes:
Powered by PublicationsList.org.